Abstract

This study examined the association between long working hours and cognitive function in middle age. Data were collected in 1997–1999 (baseline) and 2002–2004 (follow-up) from a prospective study of 2,214 British civil servants who were in full-time employment at baseline and had data on cognitive tests and covariates. A battery of cognitive tests (short-term memory, Alice Heim 4-I, Mill Hill vocabulary, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency) were measured at baseline and at follow-up. Compared with working 40 hours per week at most, working more than 55 hours per week was associated with lower scores in the vocabulary test at both baseline and follow-up. Long working hours also predicted decline in performance on the reasoning test (Alice Heim 4-I). Similar results were obtained by using working hours as a continuous variable; the associations between working hours and cognitive function were robust to adjustments for several potential confounding factors including age, sex, marital status, education, occupation, income, physical diseases, psychosocial factors, sleep disturbances, and health risk behaviors. This study shows that long working hours may have a negative effect on cognitive performance in middle age.

Long working hours are common worldwide; for example, in the European Union member states, 12%–17% of employees worked overtime in 2001 (1). Long working hours have been found to be associated with cardiovascular and immunologic reactions, reduced sleep duration, unhealthy lifestyle (2–8), and adverse health outcomes, such as cardiovascular disease, diabetes, subjective health complaints, fatigue (2–7), and depression (8). There is increasing evidence to suggest the importance of midlife risk factors for later dementia (9). Furthermore, the link between cognitive impairment and later life dementia is clearly established (10, 11). Thus, it is important to examine risk factors for poor cognition in midlife, and there is little research on the potential effects of long working hours on cognition among middle-aged persons.

A cross-sectional study of 248 automotive workers found an association between overtime work and impaired performance on tests of attention and executive function (12). This finding was in agreement with findings from other studies that focused on different forms of shift work or work schedule rather than on long working hours (13, 14). For example, deterioration in cognitive performance, including impaired grammatical reasoning and alertness, has been found in post versus pretest conditions among employees working 9- to 12-hour shifts compared with a traditional 8-hour shift (13). However, little is known about the health effects of long total working hours as opposed to long hours of shift work.

This study examined the relation between long working hours and cognitive function over a 5-year follow-up period in a large-scale, prospective occupational cohort of British civil servants (the Whitehall II study) (15). We were able to take into account several factors that may act as confounders or mediators of this association, such as education, occupational position, physical health status, psychological and psychosocial factors, sleep problems, and health risk behaviors (2).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants and procedure

The Whitehall II study sample recruitment (phase 1) took place between late 1985 and early 1988 among all office staff, aged 35–55 years, from 20 London-based Civil Service departments (15). The response rate was 73% (6,895 men and 3,413 women). Since phase 1, there have been 7 further data collection phases. Informed consent was gained from all participants. The University College London Medical School Committee on the Ethics of Human Research approved the protocol.

As cognitive performance was measured on the whole sample for the first time at phase 5, this phase is used as baseline for the present study. We included all 2,214 participants (1,694 men and 520 women) who were employed and responded to the questions on working hours and for whom the covariates and cognitive test scores were available at phase 5 (1997–1999) and phase 7 (2002–2004). A flow chart of sample selection is shown in Figure 1. The mean age of the 2,214 participants at phase 5 was 52.1 years (standard deviation, 4.2; range, 45–66). There were no major differences between the participants and all full-time employees who participated in phase 5 (n = 3,597) in terms of age (52.1 vs. 52.4 years), sex (77% vs. 75% male), occupational grade (18% with the lowest occupational grade vs. 22%), and prevalence of coronary heart disease (10% vs. 11%). However, employees who participated in our study at phases 5 and 7 differed from the cohort at recruitment to the Whitehall II study (n = 10,308), in that they were younger (mean age, 40.6 vs. 44.5 years at phase 1); more likely to be male (77% vs. 67%) and from the higher socioeconomic groups (10% with the lowest grade vs. 23%); and less likely to have preexisting coronary heart disease at phase 1 (2.7% vs. 4.1%).

Figure 1.

Sample selection, the Whitehall II study, 1997–2004. TIA, transient ischemic attack.

Figure 1.

Sample selection, the Whitehall II study, 1997–2004. TIA, transient ischemic attack.

Tests of cognitive function

The cognitive function test battery at phases 5 and 7 consisted of 5 standard tasks chosen to evaluate cognitive functioning in middle-aged adults. The first was verbal memory assessed by a 20-word free recall test of short-term memory. Participants were presented a list of 20 one- or two-syllable words at 2-second intervals and were then asked to recall in writing as many of the words in any order within 2 minutes. The Alice Heim 4-I (AH 4-I) test (16) is a measure of inductive reasoning that assesses fluid intelligence, that is, the ability to identify patterns and to infer principles and rules. This test is composed of a series of 65 items (32 verbal and 33 mathematical reasoning items) of increasing difficulty. The participants had 10 minutes to complete this section. The Mill Hill vocabulary test (17) assesses crystallized intelligence, that is, knowledge of verbal meaning, and encompasses the ability to recognize and comprehend words. We used this test in its multiple-choice format that consists of a list of 33 stimulus words ordered by increasing difficulty, with 6 response choices per word. The final 2 tests were measures of verbal fluency: phonemic and semantic (18). Phonemic fluency was assessed via “S” words, and semantic fluency was assessed via “animal” words. Subjects were asked to recall in writing as many words beginning with “S” and as many animal names as they could. One minute was allowed for each test of verbal fluency. A higher score indicated better performance in each test.

The change score was calculated for each measure of cognitive function as phase 7 score minus phase 5 score. As the time interval between clinical examination at phases 5 and 7 varied between 3.9 and 7.1 years (mean, 5.5 years), the difference in cognitive score was divided by the time in years between the 2 measures for each individual and multiplied by 5 to give everyone the same (5-year) time period between the 2 phases of cognitive data collection.

Working hours and other baseline characteristics

Working hours were determined at phase 5 from the following 2 questions: “How many hours do you work per average week in your main job including work brought home?” and “How many hours do you work in an average week in your additional employment?”. Participants were divided into the following 3 groups: a total of 35–40 hours; 41–55 hours; and more than 55 hours per week (5–7). In addition, analyses were conducted by using the scale as a continuous variable. Participants in the Whitehall II study are almost exclusively white-collar civil servants. The most common weekly working hours correspond to 36 hours per week net, although various flexible working arrangements can also be arranged. In the present cohort, the total mean working hours were 45.2 hours/week (standard deviation, 8.0; range, 35–120).

Altogether, 20 sociodemographic characteristics and behavioral, psychological, psychosocial, and medical conditions known to be associated with cognitive function and/or working hours were included as covariates in the analysis (2–9, 12, 19–38). In addition to sex and age, marital status, indicators of socioeconomic position, that is, occupational grade (6 levels from which the lowest 2 levels were collapsed to obtain sufficient numbers), education (postgraduate, graduate, higher secondary school, lower secondary school, or no academic qualifications), and the participant's report of his/her annual gross salary were assessed. Employment status (working vs. not working) at follow-up was obtained from the phase 7 questionnaire.

The physical functioning component score of the Medical Outcomes Study SF-36 test (39) was used as a measure of global physical health status and divided into quartiles separately for men and women. Prevalent coronary heart disease at phase 5 included cases of nonfatal myocardial infarction and angina. In addition to definite nonfatal myocardial infarction and definite angina, our total nonfatal coronary heart disease events outcome included self-reported cases in the absence of any clinical record evidence of coronary disease. Systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure were measured by using a Hawksley random-zero sphygmomanometer (Hawksley and Sons, Ltd., Lancing, United Kingdom). In keeping with standard definitions, subjects with systolic blood pressure of ≥140 mm Hg and diastolic blood pressure of ≥90 mm Hg or on antihypertensive treatment were considered to be hypertensive (40).

Psychological distress was assessed by using the 30-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-30) (41). The GHQ-30 has been validated in a number of diverse populations and has been validated specifically against the Clinical Interview Schedule in Whitehall II data, giving a cutoff point of 4/5 positive responses for dividing noncases from cases (42). In addition, a 5-item subscale of anxiety (e.g., feelings of constant strain, panic, nervousness) was derived from the GHQ-30 (41). Scores in the top decile were used to define anxiety cases, corresponding to the prevalence of anxiety disorders in the general population (43).

Sleep was assessed in 2 ways; the first was a measure of duration with respondents identified as short sleepers if they reported sleeping less than 6 hours on an average week night (44). Sleep quality was assessed by using the “Jenkins scale” (45), which assesses sleep disturbances during the past 4 weeks. The mean response score for all 4 questions was divided into tertiles.

Of the health behaviors, alcohol consumption (units/week) was classified into 3 categories: none; >0–14 (women)/21 (men) units; >14/>21 units (46). Smoking was assessed by a single question of whether the respondent was a current smoker or not. For the physical activity score, the participants were asked about the frequency and duration of their participation in physical activity (47). The amount of time spent in activities with metabolic equivalent values ranging from 0 to 6 or above was summed to allow calculation of the total number of hours per week of physical activity and divided into 3 categories—low, moderate, and high.

Social support was measured by the 15-item Close Persons Questionnaire (48), which includes questions about confiding/emotional support, practical support, and negative aspects of close relationships. The mean of all responses was divided into tertiles. Strain in family relations was measured with a single-item question of how often the participant had any worries or problems with other relatives, for example, parents or in-laws (always/often vs. sometimes/seldom/never/not applicable). Job strain was formulated by splitting the job demands score and decision latitude score at their medians. High demands and low decision latitude indicated high job strain, and other combinations indicated low job strain (49).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were carried out by using SAS, version 9.1, statistical software (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina), except missing-data analysis which was done using STATA, version 9.0, statistical software (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas). First, we compared baseline characteristics of the participants by working hours and compared the longer-hours group (>55 hours per week) with the employees with normal working hours (35–40 hours per week) using χ2 tests. We used multiple analysis of covariance to examine whether work hours had an overall association with cognitive function, as checking for each measure of cognitive function separately increases the chance of Type 1 error. Subsequently, analysis of variance was used to assess the association between work hours and individual measures of cognitive function. When a significant difference was found in cognitive function tests at baseline and/or at follow-up between groups, additional analyses were carried out with the change score to assess temporal order and to examine whether the change was statistically significant. Sequential analyses were undertaken to see whether adjustment for covariates attenuated the association between long working hours and change in cognitive function. Age was entered into the models as a continuous variable, and all other covariates were entered as categorical variables. As recommended by Glymour et al. (50), we used baseline-unadjusted change scores for cognitive change. In order to examine linear trend in the association between working hours and cognitive function, we repeated the analysis using working hours as a continuous variable.

To explore whether selection bias might have occurred because of loss to follow-up, we undertook a sensitivity analysis in which we used multiple multivariate imputation (51) using working hours, all covariates, and cognition variables to impute values for missing values in any variables with some missing data, among all 3,163 participants free of stroke and transient ischemic attack at baseline. We used switching regression in STATA software, as described by Royston (51), carried out 20 cycles of regression switching, and generated 20 imputation data sets. The multiple multivariate imputation approach creates a number of copies of the data (in this case, we generated 20 copies), each of which has values that are missing imputed with an appropriate level of randomness using chained equations. The estimates are obtained by averaging across the results from each of these 20 data sets using Rubin's rules. The procedure takes account of uncertainty in the imputation, as well as uncertainty due to random variation, as undertaken in all multivariable analyses.

RESULTS

Characteristics of the study participants by working hours at baseline are shown in Table 1. A total of 853 (39%) participants reported 35–40 hours of work per week, 1,180 (53%) reported 41–55 hours, and 181 (8%) reported more than 55 hours of work per week. Compared with employees with 35–40 hours, a higher percentage of those who worked more than 55 hours were men and were married or cohabited and had a higher occupational grade, higher education, higher income, more psychological distress, shorter sleep, higher alcohol use, and more social support.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Participants by Working Hours at Baseline, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

 All Working Hours per Week
 
P Valuea 
 ≤40
 
41–55
 
>55
 
 No. No. No. No. 
Sex          
    Men 1,694 77 607 71 936 79 151 83 <0.001 
    Women 520 23 246 29 244 21 30 17 
Age, mean years (SE) 52.1 (0.09) 52.4 (0.14) 51.8 (0.12) 52.5 (0.31) 0.741 
Marital status          
    Married/cohabited 1,749 79 624 73 969 82 156 86 <0.001 
    Nonmarried/noncohabited 465 21 229 27 211 18 25 14 
Occupational grade level          
    1 (highest) 495 22 75 328 28 92 51 <0.001 
    2 569 26 164 19 356 30 49 27 
    3 359 16 161 19 180 15 18 10 
    4 400 18 231 27 160 14 
    5–6 (lowest) 391 18 222 26 156 13 13 
Educational level          
    Postgraduate 378 17 93 11 238 20 47 26 <0.001 
    Graduate 551 25 200 23 294 25 57 31 
    Higher secondary 657 30 251 29 356 30 50 28 
    Lower secondary 499 23 240 28 235 20 24 13 
    No academic qualifications 129 69 57 
Income, £/year          
    ≥50,000 358 16 46 232 20 80 44 <0.001 
    25,000–<50,000 1,176 53 393 46 703 60 80 44 
    15,000–<25,000 579 26 351 41 214 18 14 
    <15,000 101 63 31 
Physical health status          
    I (lowest) 404 18 148 17 215 18 41 23 0.137 
    II 551 25 219 26 298 25 34 19 
    III 589 27 234 27 301 26 54 30 
    IV (highest) 670 30 252 30 366 31 52 29 
Coronary heart disease          
    No 1,988 90 769 90 1,059 90 160 88 0.478 
    Yes 226 10 84 10 121 10 21 12 
Hypertension          
    No 1,801 81 682 80 969 82 150 83 0.368 
    Yes 413 19 171 20 211 18 31 17 
Psychological distress          
    No 1,674 76 678 79 880 75 116 64 <0.001 
    Yes 540 24 175 21 300 25 65 36 
Anxiety          
    No 1,972 89 770 90 1,044 88 158 87 0.230 
    Yes 242 11 83 10 136 12 23 13 
Short sleep (<6 hours)          
    No 2,043 92 799 94 1,084 92 160 88 0.013 
    Yes 171 54 96 21 12 
Sleeping problems          
    Low 707 32 292 34 360 31 55 30 0.375 
    Intermediate 836 38 316 37 455 39 65 36 
    High 671 30 245 29 365 31 61 34 
Alcohol use          
    No 259 12 118 14 127 11 14 0.018 
    Moderate 1,382 62 553 65 714 61 115 64 
    High 573 26 182 21 339 29 52 29 
Smoking          
    No 2,020 91 777 91 1,076 91 167 92 0.611 
    Yes 194 76 104 14 
Physical activity          
    Low 352 16 153 18 164 14 35 19 0.819 
    Intermediate 780 35 326 38 389 33 65 36 
    High 1,082 49 374 44 627 53 81 45 
Social support          
    Low 763 34 312 37 406 34 45 25 0.008 
    Intermediate 764 35 306 36 386 33 72 40 
    High 687 31 235 28 388 33 64 35 
Strain in family relations          
    No 1,792 81 700 82 951 81 141 78 0.192 
    Yes 422 19 153 18 229 19 40 22 
Job strain          
    No 1,671 75 648 76 886 75 137 76 0.937 
    Yes 543 25 205 24 294 25 44 24 
Employment status at follow-up          
    Employed 1,680 76 624 73 911 77 36 80 0.052 
    Nonemployed 534 24 229 27 269 23 145 20 
 All Working Hours per Week
 
P Valuea 
 ≤40
 
41–55
 
>55
 
 No. No. No. No. 
Sex          
    Men 1,694 77 607 71 936 79 151 83 <0.001 
    Women 520 23 246 29 244 21 30 17 
Age, mean years (SE) 52.1 (0.09) 52.4 (0.14) 51.8 (0.12) 52.5 (0.31) 0.741 
Marital status          
    Married/cohabited 1,749 79 624 73 969 82 156 86 <0.001 
    Nonmarried/noncohabited 465 21 229 27 211 18 25 14 
Occupational grade level          
    1 (highest) 495 22 75 328 28 92 51 <0.001 
    2 569 26 164 19 356 30 49 27 
    3 359 16 161 19 180 15 18 10 
    4 400 18 231 27 160 14 
    5–6 (lowest) 391 18 222 26 156 13 13 
Educational level          
    Postgraduate 378 17 93 11 238 20 47 26 <0.001 
    Graduate 551 25 200 23 294 25 57 31 
    Higher secondary 657 30 251 29 356 30 50 28 
    Lower secondary 499 23 240 28 235 20 24 13 
    No academic qualifications 129 69 57 
Income, £/year          
    ≥50,000 358 16 46 232 20 80 44 <0.001 
    25,000–<50,000 1,176 53 393 46 703 60 80 44 
    15,000–<25,000 579 26 351 41 214 18 14 
    <15,000 101 63 31 
Physical health status          
    I (lowest) 404 18 148 17 215 18 41 23 0.137 
    II 551 25 219 26 298 25 34 19 
    III 589 27 234 27 301 26 54 30 
    IV (highest) 670 30 252 30 366 31 52 29 
Coronary heart disease          
    No 1,988 90 769 90 1,059 90 160 88 0.478 
    Yes 226 10 84 10 121 10 21 12 
Hypertension          
    No 1,801 81 682 80 969 82 150 83 0.368 
    Yes 413 19 171 20 211 18 31 17 
Psychological distress          
    No 1,674 76 678 79 880 75 116 64 <0.001 
    Yes 540 24 175 21 300 25 65 36 
Anxiety          
    No 1,972 89 770 90 1,044 88 158 87 0.230 
    Yes 242 11 83 10 136 12 23 13 
Short sleep (<6 hours)          
    No 2,043 92 799 94 1,084 92 160 88 0.013 
    Yes 171 54 96 21 12 
Sleeping problems          
    Low 707 32 292 34 360 31 55 30 0.375 
    Intermediate 836 38 316 37 455 39 65 36 
    High 671 30 245 29 365 31 61 34 
Alcohol use          
    No 259 12 118 14 127 11 14 0.018 
    Moderate 1,382 62 553 65 714 61 115 64 
    High 573 26 182 21 339 29 52 29 
Smoking          
    No 2,020 91 777 91 1,076 91 167 92 0.611 
    Yes 194 76 104 14 
Physical activity          
    Low 352 16 153 18 164 14 35 19 0.819 
    Intermediate 780 35 326 38 389 33 65 36 
    High 1,082 49 374 44 627 53 81 45 
Social support          
    Low 763 34 312 37 406 34 45 25 0.008 
    Intermediate 764 35 306 36 386 33 72 40 
    High 687 31 235 28 388 33 64 35 
Strain in family relations          
    No 1,792 81 700 82 951 81 141 78 0.192 
    Yes 422 19 153 18 229 19 40 22 
Job strain          
    No 1,671 75 648 76 886 75 137 76 0.937 
    Yes 543 25 205 24 294 25 44 24 
Employment status at follow-up          
    Employed 1,680 76 624 73 911 77 36 80 0.052 
    Nonemployed 534 24 229 27 269 23 145 20 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

P value for difference between the groups working 40 hours or less and those working more than 55 hours per week.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Participants by Working Hours at Baseline, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

 All Working Hours per Week
 
P Valuea 
 ≤40
 
41–55
 
>55
 
 No. No. No. No. 
Sex          
    Men 1,694 77 607 71 936 79 151 83 <0.001 
    Women 520 23 246 29 244 21 30 17 
Age, mean years (SE) 52.1 (0.09) 52.4 (0.14) 51.8 (0.12) 52.5 (0.31) 0.741 
Marital status          
    Married/cohabited 1,749 79 624 73 969 82 156 86 <0.001 
    Nonmarried/noncohabited 465 21 229 27 211 18 25 14 
Occupational grade level          
    1 (highest) 495 22 75 328 28 92 51 <0.001 
    2 569 26 164 19 356 30 49 27 
    3 359 16 161 19 180 15 18 10 
    4 400 18 231 27 160 14 
    5–6 (lowest) 391 18 222 26 156 13 13 
Educational level          
    Postgraduate 378 17 93 11 238 20 47 26 <0.001 
    Graduate 551 25 200 23 294 25 57 31 
    Higher secondary 657 30 251 29 356 30 50 28 
    Lower secondary 499 23 240 28 235 20 24 13 
    No academic qualifications 129 69 57 
Income, £/year          
    ≥50,000 358 16 46 232 20 80 44 <0.001 
    25,000–<50,000 1,176 53 393 46 703 60 80 44 
    15,000–<25,000 579 26 351 41 214 18 14 
    <15,000 101 63 31 
Physical health status          
    I (lowest) 404 18 148 17 215 18 41 23 0.137 
    II 551 25 219 26 298 25 34 19 
    III 589 27 234 27 301 26 54 30 
    IV (highest) 670 30 252 30 366 31 52 29 
Coronary heart disease          
    No 1,988 90 769 90 1,059 90 160 88 0.478 
    Yes 226 10 84 10 121 10 21 12 
Hypertension          
    No 1,801 81 682 80 969 82 150 83 0.368 
    Yes 413 19 171 20 211 18 31 17 
Psychological distress          
    No 1,674 76 678 79 880 75 116 64 <0.001 
    Yes 540 24 175 21 300 25 65 36 
Anxiety          
    No 1,972 89 770 90 1,044 88 158 87 0.230 
    Yes 242 11 83 10 136 12 23 13 
Short sleep (<6 hours)          
    No 2,043 92 799 94 1,084 92 160 88 0.013 
    Yes 171 54 96 21 12 
Sleeping problems          
    Low 707 32 292 34 360 31 55 30 0.375 
    Intermediate 836 38 316 37 455 39 65 36 
    High 671 30 245 29 365 31 61 34 
Alcohol use          
    No 259 12 118 14 127 11 14 0.018 
    Moderate 1,382 62 553 65 714 61 115 64 
    High 573 26 182 21 339 29 52 29 
Smoking          
    No 2,020 91 777 91 1,076 91 167 92 0.611 
    Yes 194 76 104 14 
Physical activity          
    Low 352 16 153 18 164 14 35 19 0.819 
    Intermediate 780 35 326 38 389 33 65 36 
    High 1,082 49 374 44 627 53 81 45 
Social support          
    Low 763 34 312 37 406 34 45 25 0.008 
    Intermediate 764 35 306 36 386 33 72 40 
    High 687 31 235 28 388 33 64 35 
Strain in family relations          
    No 1,792 81 700 82 951 81 141 78 0.192 
    Yes 422 19 153 18 229 19 40 22 
Job strain          
    No 1,671 75 648 76 886 75 137 76 0.937 
    Yes 543 25 205 24 294 25 44 24 
Employment status at follow-up          
    Employed 1,680 76 624 73 911 77 36 80 0.052 
    Nonemployed 534 24 229 27 269 23 145 20 
 All Working Hours per Week
 
P Valuea 
 ≤40
 
41–55
 
>55
 
 No. No. No. No. 
Sex          
    Men 1,694 77 607 71 936 79 151 83 <0.001 
    Women 520 23 246 29 244 21 30 17 
Age, mean years (SE) 52.1 (0.09) 52.4 (0.14) 51.8 (0.12) 52.5 (0.31) 0.741 
Marital status          
    Married/cohabited 1,749 79 624 73 969 82 156 86 <0.001 
    Nonmarried/noncohabited 465 21 229 27 211 18 25 14 
Occupational grade level          
    1 (highest) 495 22 75 328 28 92 51 <0.001 
    2 569 26 164 19 356 30 49 27 
    3 359 16 161 19 180 15 18 10 
    4 400 18 231 27 160 14 
    5–6 (lowest) 391 18 222 26 156 13 13 
Educational level          
    Postgraduate 378 17 93 11 238 20 47 26 <0.001 
    Graduate 551 25 200 23 294 25 57 31 
    Higher secondary 657 30 251 29 356 30 50 28 
    Lower secondary 499 23 240 28 235 20 24 13 
    No academic qualifications 129 69 57 
Income, £/year          
    ≥50,000 358 16 46 232 20 80 44 <0.001 
    25,000–<50,000 1,176 53 393 46 703 60 80 44 
    15,000–<25,000 579 26 351 41 214 18 14 
    <15,000 101 63 31 
Physical health status          
    I (lowest) 404 18 148 17 215 18 41 23 0.137 
    II 551 25 219 26 298 25 34 19 
    III 589 27 234 27 301 26 54 30 
    IV (highest) 670 30 252 30 366 31 52 29 
Coronary heart disease          
    No 1,988 90 769 90 1,059 90 160 88 0.478 
    Yes 226 10 84 10 121 10 21 12 
Hypertension          
    No 1,801 81 682 80 969 82 150 83 0.368 
    Yes 413 19 171 20 211 18 31 17 
Psychological distress          
    No 1,674 76 678 79 880 75 116 64 <0.001 
    Yes 540 24 175 21 300 25 65 36 
Anxiety          
    No 1,972 89 770 90 1,044 88 158 87 0.230 
    Yes 242 11 83 10 136 12 23 13 
Short sleep (<6 hours)          
    No 2,043 92 799 94 1,084 92 160 88 0.013 
    Yes 171 54 96 21 12 
Sleeping problems          
    Low 707 32 292 34 360 31 55 30 0.375 
    Intermediate 836 38 316 37 455 39 65 36 
    High 671 30 245 29 365 31 61 34 
Alcohol use          
    No 259 12 118 14 127 11 14 0.018 
    Moderate 1,382 62 553 65 714 61 115 64 
    High 573 26 182 21 339 29 52 29 
Smoking          
    No 2,020 91 777 91 1,076 91 167 92 0.611 
    Yes 194 76 104 14 
Physical activity          
    Low 352 16 153 18 164 14 35 19 0.819 
    Intermediate 780 35 326 38 389 33 65 36 
    High 1,082 49 374 44 627 53 81 45 
Social support          
    Low 763 34 312 37 406 34 45 25 0.008 
    Intermediate 764 35 306 36 386 33 72 40 
    High 687 31 235 28 388 33 64 35 
Strain in family relations          
    No 1,792 81 700 82 951 81 141 78 0.192 
    Yes 422 19 153 18 229 19 40 22 
Job strain          
    No 1,671 75 648 76 886 75 137 76 0.937 
    Yes 543 25 205 24 294 25 44 24 
Employment status at follow-up          
    Employed 1,680 76 624 73 911 77 36 80 0.052 
    Nonemployed 534 24 229 27 269 23 145 20 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

P value for difference between the groups working 40 hours or less and those working more than 55 hours per week.

Multiple analysis of covariance revealed an overall association of working hours with cognitive function at baseline (P = 0.002) and follow-up (P = 0.037), as well as change in cognitive function scores between baseline and follow-up (P = 0.044). Table 2 shows the associations between working hours at baseline and each cognitive function measure at baseline and at follow-up after adjustment for all the covariates measured at baseline. Compared with employees working 40 hours or less per week, employees working more than 55 hours had lower vocabulary scores at baseline and at follow-up. At follow-up, they had lower scores also on the reasoning test. No significant difference between groups was found in any other measures of cognitive function at follow-up. Repeating these analyses with working hours treated as a continuous variable largely replicated the findings and additionally showed an association between working hours and better phonemic fluency at baseline but not at follow-up.

Table 2.

Association Between Working Hours at Baseline and Cognitive Function at Baseline and at Follow-up, Fully Adjusted Models,a the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004b

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Memory Range, 0–18c (1–18)d
 
Reasoning Range, 12–65c (10–65)d
 
Vocabulary Range, 1–33c (6–32)d
 
Phonemic Fluency Range, 3–47c (2–34)d
 
Semantic Fluency Range, 2–34c (2–33)d
 
Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee 
 Cognitive function at baseline 
≤40 6.94 (0.18) Referent 46.14 (0.63) Referent 24.80 (0.25) Referent 16.95 (0.33) Referent 16.83 (0.30) Referent 
41–55 7.12 (0.17) 0.081 46.02 (0.60) 0.744 24.38 (0.24) 0.005 17.25 (0.31) 0.117 16.87 (0.29) 0.810 
>55 7.14 (0.23) 0.306 45.93 (0.79) 0.763 23.96 (0.32) 0.002 17.62 (0.41) 0.056 17.08 (0.38) 0.441 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.835 P = 0.206 P < 0.001 P = 0.031 P = 0.874 
 Cognitive function at follow-up 
≤40 7.11 (0.18) Referent 44.17 (0.65) Referent 24.97 (0.25) Referent 15.66 (0.31) Referent 16.20 (0.28) Referent 
41–55 7.18 (0.18) 0.547 43.53 (0.62) 0.099 24.62 (0.24) 0.020 15.95 (0.29) 0.111 16.18 (0.26) 0.912 
>55 6.93 (0.23) 0.359 42.74 (0.81) 0.040 24.39 (0.32) 0.032 16.00 (0.38) 0.302 16.08 (0.35) 0.680 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.118 P = 0.010 P = 0.003 P = 0.088 P = 0.430 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Memory Range, 0–18c (1–18)d
 
Reasoning Range, 12–65c (10–65)d
 
Vocabulary Range, 1–33c (6–32)d
 
Phonemic Fluency Range, 3–47c (2–34)d
 
Semantic Fluency Range, 2–34c (2–33)d
 
Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee 
 Cognitive function at baseline 
≤40 6.94 (0.18) Referent 46.14 (0.63) Referent 24.80 (0.25) Referent 16.95 (0.33) Referent 16.83 (0.30) Referent 
41–55 7.12 (0.17) 0.081 46.02 (0.60) 0.744 24.38 (0.24) 0.005 17.25 (0.31) 0.117 16.87 (0.29) 0.810 
>55 7.14 (0.23) 0.306 45.93 (0.79) 0.763 23.96 (0.32) 0.002 17.62 (0.41) 0.056 17.08 (0.38) 0.441 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.835 P = 0.206 P < 0.001 P = 0.031 P = 0.874 
 Cognitive function at follow-up 
≤40 7.11 (0.18) Referent 44.17 (0.65) Referent 24.97 (0.25) Referent 15.66 (0.31) Referent 16.20 (0.28) Referent 
41–55 7.18 (0.18) 0.547 43.53 (0.62) 0.099 24.62 (0.24) 0.020 15.95 (0.29) 0.111 16.18 (0.26) 0.912 
>55 6.93 (0.23) 0.359 42.74 (0.81) 0.040 24.39 (0.32) 0.032 16.00 (0.38) 0.302 16.08 (0.35) 0.680 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.118 P = 0.010 P = 0.003 P = 0.088 P = 0.430 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Adjusted for age, sex, marital status, follow-up employment status, occupational grade, education, income, physical health indicators, psychological distress, anxiety, sleep problems, health risk behaviors, social support, family stress, and job strain.

b

In each cognitive test, a higher score indicates better cognitive performance.

c

Range of scores at baseline.

d

Range of scores at follow-up.

e

P value for difference with the referent group working 40 hours or less per week.

f

Total working hours entered into the model as a continuous variable.

Table 2.

Association Between Working Hours at Baseline and Cognitive Function at Baseline and at Follow-up, Fully Adjusted Models,a the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004b

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Memory Range, 0–18c (1–18)d
 
Reasoning Range, 12–65c (10–65)d
 
Vocabulary Range, 1–33c (6–32)d
 
Phonemic Fluency Range, 3–47c (2–34)d
 
Semantic Fluency Range, 2–34c (2–33)d
 
Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee 
 Cognitive function at baseline 
≤40 6.94 (0.18) Referent 46.14 (0.63) Referent 24.80 (0.25) Referent 16.95 (0.33) Referent 16.83 (0.30) Referent 
41–55 7.12 (0.17) 0.081 46.02 (0.60) 0.744 24.38 (0.24) 0.005 17.25 (0.31) 0.117 16.87 (0.29) 0.810 
>55 7.14 (0.23) 0.306 45.93 (0.79) 0.763 23.96 (0.32) 0.002 17.62 (0.41) 0.056 17.08 (0.38) 0.441 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.835 P = 0.206 P < 0.001 P = 0.031 P = 0.874 
 Cognitive function at follow-up 
≤40 7.11 (0.18) Referent 44.17 (0.65) Referent 24.97 (0.25) Referent 15.66 (0.31) Referent 16.20 (0.28) Referent 
41–55 7.18 (0.18) 0.547 43.53 (0.62) 0.099 24.62 (0.24) 0.020 15.95 (0.29) 0.111 16.18 (0.26) 0.912 
>55 6.93 (0.23) 0.359 42.74 (0.81) 0.040 24.39 (0.32) 0.032 16.00 (0.38) 0.302 16.08 (0.35) 0.680 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.118 P = 0.010 P = 0.003 P = 0.088 P = 0.430 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Memory Range, 0–18c (1–18)d
 
Reasoning Range, 12–65c (10–65)d
 
Vocabulary Range, 1–33c (6–32)d
 
Phonemic Fluency Range, 3–47c (2–34)d
 
Semantic Fluency Range, 2–34c (2–33)d
 
Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee Mean (SE) P Valuee 
 Cognitive function at baseline 
≤40 6.94 (0.18) Referent 46.14 (0.63) Referent 24.80 (0.25) Referent 16.95 (0.33) Referent 16.83 (0.30) Referent 
41–55 7.12 (0.17) 0.081 46.02 (0.60) 0.744 24.38 (0.24) 0.005 17.25 (0.31) 0.117 16.87 (0.29) 0.810 
>55 7.14 (0.23) 0.306 45.93 (0.79) 0.763 23.96 (0.32) 0.002 17.62 (0.41) 0.056 17.08 (0.38) 0.441 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.835 P = 0.206 P < 0.001 P = 0.031 P = 0.874 
 Cognitive function at follow-up 
≤40 7.11 (0.18) Referent 44.17 (0.65) Referent 24.97 (0.25) Referent 15.66 (0.31) Referent 16.20 (0.28) Referent 
41–55 7.18 (0.18) 0.547 43.53 (0.62) 0.099 24.62 (0.24) 0.020 15.95 (0.29) 0.111 16.18 (0.26) 0.912 
>55 6.93 (0.23) 0.359 42.74 (0.81) 0.040 24.39 (0.32) 0.032 16.00 (0.38) 0.302 16.08 (0.35) 0.680 
    Test for linear trendf P = 0.118 P = 0.010 P = 0.003 P = 0.088 P = 0.430 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Adjusted for age, sex, marital status, follow-up employment status, occupational grade, education, income, physical health indicators, psychological distress, anxiety, sleep problems, health risk behaviors, social support, family stress, and job strain.

b

In each cognitive test, a higher score indicates better cognitive performance.

c

Range of scores at baseline.

d

Range of scores at follow-up.

e

P value for difference with the referent group working 40 hours or less per week.

f

Total working hours entered into the model as a continuous variable.

Table 3 examines the mean difference in the change in reasoning score between those working normal hours and those working long hours. Successive models show the effects of step-by-step adjustments. The stepwise adjustments show that various adjustments produced little attenuation of the effect of working hours on the decline in reasoning score, and a clear dose-response pattern was revealed between exposure and outcome. Again, the findings were replicated in models replacing categories with a continuous measure of working hours.

Table 3.

Stepwise Adjustmentsa of the Association Between Working Hours at Baseline and Change in Reasoning Score Between Baseline and Follow-up, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Change in Reasoning Score
 
Model I
 
Model II
 
Model III
 
Model IV
 
Model V
 
Model VI
 
Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb 
≤40 −1.90 (0.19) Referent −1.95 (0.24) Referent −1.71 (0.28) Referent −1.94 (0.25) Referent −1.87 (0.21) Referent −1.77 (0.39) Referent 
41–55 −2.36 (0.18) 0.033 −2.41 (0.23) 0.035 −2.18 (0.27) 0.030 −2.37 (0.24) 0.049 −2.34 (0.20) 0.031 −2.23 (0.37) 0.046 
>55 −2.97 (0.38) 0.006 −2.98 (0.41) 0.009 −2.80 (0.42) 0.006 −2.99 (0.41) 0.008 −2.96 (0.39) 0.006 −2.90 (0.49) 0.007 
    Mean differencec −1.08 −1.04 −1.09 −1.05 −1.09 −1.13 
    Test for linear trendd P = 0.020 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.036 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Change in Reasoning Score
 
Model I
 
Model II
 
Model III
 
Model IV
 
Model V
 
Model VI
 
Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb 
≤40 −1.90 (0.19) Referent −1.95 (0.24) Referent −1.71 (0.28) Referent −1.94 (0.25) Referent −1.87 (0.21) Referent −1.77 (0.39) Referent 
41–55 −2.36 (0.18) 0.033 −2.41 (0.23) 0.035 −2.18 (0.27) 0.030 −2.37 (0.24) 0.049 −2.34 (0.20) 0.031 −2.23 (0.37) 0.046 
>55 −2.97 (0.38) 0.006 −2.98 (0.41) 0.009 −2.80 (0.42) 0.006 −2.99 (0.41) 0.008 −2.96 (0.39) 0.006 −2.90 (0.49) 0.007 
    Mean differencec −1.08 −1.04 −1.09 −1.05 −1.09 −1.13 
    Test for linear trendd P = 0.020 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.036 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Model I, adjusted for age, sex, marital status, and follow-up employment status. Model II, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for physical health indicators. Model III, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for psychological distress, anxiety, and sleep problems. Model IV, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for health risk behaviors. Model V, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for social support, family stress, and job strain. Model VI, adjusted for all covariates in models I, II, III, IV, and V plus occupational grade, education, and income.

b

P for difference with the referent group working 40 hours or less per week.

c

Difference in mean between the group working more than 55 hours and the group working 40 hours or less per week.

d

Total working hours entered into the model as a continuous variable.

Table 3.

Stepwise Adjustmentsa of the Association Between Working Hours at Baseline and Change in Reasoning Score Between Baseline and Follow-up, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Change in Reasoning Score
 
Model I
 
Model II
 
Model III
 
Model IV
 
Model V
 
Model VI
 
Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb 
≤40 −1.90 (0.19) Referent −1.95 (0.24) Referent −1.71 (0.28) Referent −1.94 (0.25) Referent −1.87 (0.21) Referent −1.77 (0.39) Referent 
41–55 −2.36 (0.18) 0.033 −2.41 (0.23) 0.035 −2.18 (0.27) 0.030 −2.37 (0.24) 0.049 −2.34 (0.20) 0.031 −2.23 (0.37) 0.046 
>55 −2.97 (0.38) 0.006 −2.98 (0.41) 0.009 −2.80 (0.42) 0.006 −2.99 (0.41) 0.008 −2.96 (0.39) 0.006 −2.90 (0.49) 0.007 
    Mean differencec −1.08 −1.04 −1.09 −1.05 −1.09 −1.13 
    Test for linear trendd P = 0.020 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.036 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Change in Reasoning Score
 
Model I
 
Model II
 
Model III
 
Model IV
 
Model V
 
Model VI
 
Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb Mean Difference (SE) P Valueb 
≤40 −1.90 (0.19) Referent −1.95 (0.24) Referent −1.71 (0.28) Referent −1.94 (0.25) Referent −1.87 (0.21) Referent −1.77 (0.39) Referent 
41–55 −2.36 (0.18) 0.033 −2.41 (0.23) 0.035 −2.18 (0.27) 0.030 −2.37 (0.24) 0.049 −2.34 (0.20) 0.031 −2.23 (0.37) 0.046 
>55 −2.97 (0.38) 0.006 −2.98 (0.41) 0.009 −2.80 (0.42) 0.006 −2.99 (0.41) 0.008 −2.96 (0.39) 0.006 −2.90 (0.49) 0.007 
    Mean differencec −1.08 −1.04 −1.09 −1.05 −1.09 −1.13 
    Test for linear trendd P = 0.020 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.026 P = 0.018 P = 0.036 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Model I, adjusted for age, sex, marital status, and follow-up employment status. Model II, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for physical health indicators. Model III, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for psychological distress, anxiety, and sleep problems. Model IV, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for health risk behaviors. Model V, model I adjustments plus additionally adjusted for social support, family stress, and job strain. Model VI, adjusted for all covariates in models I, II, III, IV, and V plus occupational grade, education, and income.

b

P for difference with the referent group working 40 hours or less per week.

c

Difference in mean between the group working more than 55 hours and the group working 40 hours or less per week.

d

Total working hours entered into the model as a continuous variable.

To further examine whether the findings are robust, we ran a sensitivity analysis in a subgroup of participants still employed at follow-up (n = 1,672, n = 1,677). Consistent with the main analyses, working more than 55 hours versus 40 hours or less was associated with a greater decline in the reasoning score (difference, −1.47; P = 0.002) and lower scores on the vocabulary test at baseline (difference, −0.77; P = 0.009) and at follow-up (difference, −0.60; P = 0.046). Corresponding P values for the continuous working hours were P = 0.009, P = 0.004, and P = 0.023.

To examine sex differences, we conducted altogether 15 tests of interaction between sex and continuous working hours on cognitive function outcomes and found 2 statistically significant interactions: for the vocabulary test at baseline (P = 0.015) and at follow-up (P = 0.003). Sex-stratified analysis showed a significant negative association between working hours and vocabulary score at baseline and at follow-up among men (P < 0.001) but not among women (P = 0.899 and 0.339).

Finally, Table 4 repeats the analyses on those associations that were found to be robust in Tables 2 and 3, except that the results were obtained from the multiple multivariate imputation analysis for the baseline population, a total of 3,163 participants. To simplify comparison of cohorts before and after imputations, we present the effects of working hours as per 10-hour increase in a continuous measure. Imputation had little effect on the associations with vocabulary at baseline and follow-up and with reasoning at follow-up. The association with reasoning at baseline was strengthened, but otherwise the associations were similar to those before imputation. Corresponding P values for the categorical working hours variable were as follows: Between the groups of >55 hours versus ≤40 hours, P < 0.001 for the vocabulary score at baseline and follow-up; P = 0.068 for the reasoning score at baseline; P = 0.002 for the reasoning score at follow-up; and P = 0.025 for the change score in reasoning (data not shown), thus replicating the original findings.

Table 4.

Multivariable-adjusteda Associations Among Working Hours, Vocabulary, and Reasoning for Participants Before and After Imputation of Missing Data, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Vocabulary Scoreb
 
Reasoning Scorec
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Change
 
Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value 
 Before imputation 
Per 10-hour increased −0.38 (0.09) <0.001 −0.27 (0.09) 0.003 −0.28 (0.23) 0.206 −0.60 (0.23) 0.010 −0.30 (0.14) 0.036 
 After imputatione 
Per 10-hour increased −0.43 (0.08) <0.001 −0.37 (0.09) <0.001 −0.54 (0.20) 0.005 −0.83 (0.21) <0.001 −0.28 (0.13) 0.033 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Vocabulary Scoreb
 
Reasoning Scorec
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Change
 
Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value 
 Before imputation 
Per 10-hour increased −0.38 (0.09) <0.001 −0.27 (0.09) 0.003 −0.28 (0.23) 0.206 −0.60 (0.23) 0.010 −0.30 (0.14) 0.036 
 After imputatione 
Per 10-hour increased −0.43 (0.08) <0.001 −0.37 (0.09) <0.001 −0.54 (0.20) 0.005 −0.83 (0.21) <0.001 −0.28 (0.13) 0.033 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Adjusted for age, sex, marital status, follow-up employment status, occupational grade, education, income, physical health indicators, psychological distress, anxiety, sleep problems, health risk behaviors, social support, family stress, and job strain.

b

Vocabulary score: n = 2,210 and n = 3,163 before and after imputation, respectively.

c

Reasoning score: n = 2,204 and n = 3,163 before and after imputation, respectively.

d

Continuous variable for working hours.

e

Based on multiple multivariate imputations.

Table 4.

Multivariable-adjusteda Associations Among Working Hours, Vocabulary, and Reasoning for Participants Before and After Imputation of Missing Data, the Whitehall II Study, 1997–2004

Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Vocabulary Scoreb
 
Reasoning Scorec
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Change
 
Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value 
 Before imputation 
Per 10-hour increased −0.38 (0.09) <0.001 −0.27 (0.09) 0.003 −0.28 (0.23) 0.206 −0.60 (0.23) 0.010 −0.30 (0.14) 0.036 
 After imputatione 
Per 10-hour increased −0.43 (0.08) <0.001 −0.37 (0.09) <0.001 −0.54 (0.20) 0.005 −0.83 (0.21) <0.001 −0.28 (0.13) 0.033 
Weekly Working Hours at Baseline Vocabulary Scoreb
 
Reasoning Scorec
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Baseline
 
Follow-up
 
Change
 
Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value Beta (SE) P Value 
 Before imputation 
Per 10-hour increased −0.38 (0.09) <0.001 −0.27 (0.09) 0.003 −0.28 (0.23) 0.206 −0.60 (0.23) 0.010 −0.30 (0.14) 0.036 
 After imputatione 
Per 10-hour increased −0.43 (0.08) <0.001 −0.37 (0.09) <0.001 −0.54 (0.20) 0.005 −0.83 (0.21) <0.001 −0.28 (0.13) 0.033 

Abbreviation: SE, standard error.

a

Adjusted for age, sex, marital status, follow-up employment status, occupational grade, education, income, physical health indicators, psychological distress, anxiety, sleep problems, health risk behaviors, social support, family stress, and job strain.

b

Vocabulary score: n = 2,210 and n = 3,163 before and after imputation, respectively.

c

Reasoning score: n = 2,204 and n = 3,163 before and after imputation, respectively.

d

Continuous variable for working hours.

e

Based on multiple multivariate imputations.

DISCUSSION

In this study of middle-aged men and women, working more than 55 hours per week was associated with lower scores on 2 of the 5 tests of cognitive function. Long working hours at baseline were related to poorer performance on the vocabulary test at both baseline and follow-up. Furthermore, long working hours predicted decline in performance on the reasoning test over a 5-year follow-up period. These effects were robust to adjustments for 20 potential confounding factors, such as education, occupational position, physical diseases (cardiovascular dysfunction), psychosocial stress factors, sleep problems, and health risk behaviors.

We found an association between long working hours and decline in the scores for the AH 4-I reasoning test and associations with the Mill Hill vocabulary tests at baseline and at follow-up. The AH 4-I test is also recognized as a measure of fluid intelligence, that is, executive function or “meta” cognitive ability as it integrates other cognitive processes such as memory, attention, and speed of information processing. Fluid intelligence is seen to be intrinsically associated with information processing and involves short-term memory, abstract thinking, creativity, ability to solve novel problems, and reaction time. It is the aspect of intelligence most affected by aging, biologic factors, diseases, and injuries (52, 53). Fluid intelligence usually increases up to the mid-20s, after which it gradually declines until the 60s when a more rapid decline takes place.

The Mill Hill vocabulary test measures crystallized intelligence that is assumed to accumulate during the lifespan through education, occupational and cultural experience, and exposure to culture and intellectual pursuits (52, 53). Crystallized abilities usually increase up to the sixth or seventh decade of age and may not decrease until after 80 years of age. We found the Mill Hill scores to remain relatively stable as expected for this middle-aged cohort. However, the Mill Hill scores were lower among employees with long working hours at both baseline and follow-up. This consistency with 2 separate measures with a 5-year interval suggests not only a plausible finding but also stability of the far-reaching effect of long working hours on vocabulary. We did not find an interaction effect between follow-up employment status and working hours on significant outcomes, which suggests that the associations found are not dependent on employment status at follow-up. However, people who work long hours might be exposed to a narrower variation of intellectual pursuits, that is, only to those that are related to their work tasks, and therefore might not be able to develop a wide variety of functions in crystallized intelligence measured by the test. However, reversed causality is also possible: Employees with lower cognitive ability may be more prone to work overtime than workers with good cognitive ability in order to get their work done.

Previous literature, mostly cross-sectional, suggests that long working hours are associated with various health outcomes, the strongest effects being observed for cardiovascular diseases, fatigue, and sleep disturbances (2–8). These can also be hypothesized to be mediating mechanisms for the association between long working hours and cognitive decline. Hypertension is associated with cognitive dysfunction by producing subtle disturbances in cerebral perfusion and affecting brain cell metabolism (19, 20). However, we found no evidence of an association between long working hours and hypertension or coronary heart disease, suggesting that the effect of long hours on cardiovascular dysfunction, if any, is unlikely to explain cognitive decline in this study.

Another hypothesis on mediating mechanisms links long working hours with psychological stress and poor recovery from work as indicated by sleeping problems and reduced sleep. Psychological stress has been suggested as affecting the brain via 2 neuroendocrine systems: 1) the sympathetic adrenomedullary system with the secretion of epinephrine and norepinephrine and 2) the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system with the secretion of cortisol (54). Of the few studies in the field, only 1 study has found an association between long working hours and neuroendocrinologic stress markers (55). We found that long working hours were associated with short sleep duration and psychologic distress but not with sleep disturbances. Further adjustment for these factors did not provide support for the hypothesis that psychological distress and poor recovery act as mediating mechanisms.

The third hypothesis suggests that long working hours may affect cognitive function through health risk behaviors. Evidence on the association between long working hours and unhealthy behaviors is weak, but there is stronger evidence for the relation between health behaviors and cognitive function (22–24, 26). We found that adjustment for all these health risk behaviors had no effect on the association between long working hours and cognitive function, suggesting that health risk behaviors may not be an important mediating or confounding variable.

When working hours were entered into the model as a continuous variable, we found an association between long hours and better phonemic fluency at baseline but not at follow-up. This inconsistency is also reflected in the lack of an association between the categorical working hours and phonemic fluency. More research is needed to determine whether employees with long working hours do better than other employees on tests of verbal fluency. Out of 15 analyses, we found 2 statistically significant interaction effects between working hours and sex, and sex-stratified analysis showed that long working hours were associated with poorer vocabulary performance among men but not among women. However, further research with larger samples is needed to examine potential sex differences in the association between working hours and cognition.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study include a large sample size and the possibility to explore prospectively the association between long working hours and a possible change in cognitive function over a 5-year interval, which has not been feasible in earlier studies. Furthermore, we used 5 separate measures of cognitive function, allowing associations with specific aspects of cognition to be observed, and we were able to adjust for a large number of covariates as potential confounding or mediating factors between the exposure and outcome.

There are also important limitations in this study. First, the period of 5 years for cognitive decline might not be sufficient to detect a significant decline in cognitive function in general. Second, the Whitehall II cohort is based on civil servants and not representative of the entire working population, limiting the generalizability of our results. Third, we used self-reported working hours, with inherent problems of recall. Fourth, middle-aged occupational cohorts, such as ours, are subject to a healthy survivor effect as the study design involves participants who are employed and gradually excludes those who develop work disability. However, all cohort studies focusing on work-related exposures at midlife are open to health-related selection because participants need to be employed. Because poor health is linked with worse cognition, the healthy survivor effect is likely to lead to conservative estimates of the associations found. The baseline of the present study was approximately 15 years after inclusion into the Whitehall II study; men, employees in the higher occupational grades, and those free from coronary heart disease were slightly overrepresented. However, the associations among work hours, vocabulary, and reasoning were robust to adjustments for sex, occupational grade, and health. Furthermore, the similarity of these associations in the complete case and multiple imputation analyses suggests that loss to follow-up after the baseline is an unlikely source of bias in this study.

Conclusions

Decline in cognitive function has already been shown to be present among the middle aged (9). As mild cognitive impairment predicts dementia (10, 11) and mortality (56–58), the identification of risk factors for mild cognitive impairment in middle age is important. The results of this study show that long working hours may be one of the risk factors that have a negative effect on cognitive performance in middle age. Our findings can have clinical significance, as the 0.6- to 1.4-unit difference in aspects of cognitive functioning between employees working long hours and those working normal hours is similar in magnitude to that of smoking, a risk factor for dementia (59), which has been found to affect cognition in the Whitehall II study (60). However, further research is needed to identify the potential underlying factors for the relation between long working hours and cognitive function and to examine the generalizability of our findings.

Abbreviations

    Abbreviations
     
  • AH 4-I

    Alice Heim 4-I

  •  
  • GHQ-30

    30-item General Health Questionnaire

Author affiliations: Centre of Expertise for Work Organizations, Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Helsinki, Finland (Marianna Virtanen, Markus Jokela, Jussi Vahtera, Mika Kivimäki); Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom (Jane E. Ferrie, Archana Singh-Manoux, David Gimeno, Michael G. Marmot, Mika Kivimäki); INSERM, Saint-Maurice, Cédex, France (Archana Singh-Manoux); Centre de Gérontologie, Hôpital Ste Périne, Paris, France (Archana Singh-Manoux); Division of Environmental and Occupational Health Sciences, Health Science Center at Houston, The University of Texas School of Public Health, San Antonio, Texas, (David Gimeno); and the National Research and Development Centre for Welfare and Health, Helsinki, Finland (Marko Elovainio).

The Whitehall II study has been supported by grants from the British Medical Research Council; the British Heart Foundation; the British Health and Safety Executive; the British Department of Health; the US National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grant HL36310); the US National Institute on Aging (grant AG13196); the US Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (grant HS06516); and the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Networks on Successful Midlife Development and Socioeconomic Status and Health. A. S-M. is supported by a “European Young Investigator Award” from the European Science Foundation. M. G. M. is supported by a British Medical Research Council research professorship. J. E. F. is supported by the British Medical Research Council (grant G8802774). J. V. and M. K. are supported by the Academy of Finland (projects 117604, 124322, and 124271).

Conflict of interest: none declared.

References

1.
Vaguer
C
Van Bastelaer
A
Working overtime
Statistics in Focus, Population and Social Conditions
 , 
2004
Dublin, Ireland
European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions
2.
van der Hulst
M
Long workhours and health
Scand J Work Environ Health
 , 
2003
, vol. 
29
 
3
(pg. 
171
-
188
)
3.
Caruso
CC
Hitchcock
EM
Dick
RB
, et al. 
Overtime and Extended Work Shifts: Recent Findings on Illnesses, Injuries, and Health Behaviors
 , 
2004
Washington, DC
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Department of Health and Human Services
4.
Johnson
JV
Lipscomb
J
Long working hours, occupational health and the changing nature of work organization
Am J Ind Med
 , 
2006
, vol. 
49
 
11
(pg. 
921
-
929
)
5.
Sokejima
S
Kagamimori
S
Working hours as a risk factor for acute myocardial infarction in Japan: case-control study
BMJ
 , 
1998
, vol. 
317
 
7161
(pg. 
775
-
780
)
6.
Liu
Y
Tanaka
H
the Fukuoka Heart Study Group. Overtime work, insufficient sleep, and risk of non-fatal acute myocardial infarction in Japanese men
Occup Environ Med
 , 
2002
, vol. 
59
 
7
(pg. 
447
-
451
)
7.
Sekine
M
Chandola
T
Martikainen
P
, et al. 
Work and family characteristics as determinants of socioeconomic and sex inequalities in sleep: the Japanese Civil Servants Study
Sleep
 , 
2006
, vol. 
29
 
2
(pg. 
206
-
216
)
8.
Shields
M
Long working hours and health
Health Rep
 , 
1999
, vol. 
11
 
2
(pg. 
33
-
48
)
9.
Kivipelto
M
Ngandu
T
Laatikainen
T
, et al. 
Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged people: a longitudinal, population-based study
Lancet Neurol
 , 
2006
, vol. 
5
 
9
(pg. 
735
-
741
)
10.
Chertkow
H
Mild cognitive impairment
Curr Opin Neurol
 , 
2002
, vol. 
15
 
4
(pg. 
401
-
407
)
11.
Morris
JC
Storandt
M
Miller
JP
, et al. 
Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease
Arch Neurol
 , 
2001
, vol. 
58
 
3
(pg. 
397
-
405
)
12.
Proctor
SP
White
RF
Robins
TG
, et al. 
Effect of overtime work on cognitive function in automotive workers
Scand J Work Environ Health
 , 
1996
, vol. 
22
 
2
(pg. 
124
-
132
)
13.
Lockley
SW
Cronin
JW
Evans
EE
, et al. 
Effect of reducing interns’ weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures
N Engl J Med
 , 
2004
, vol. 
351
 
18
(pg. 
1829
-
1837
)
14.
Knauth
P
Extended work periods
Ind Health
 , 
2007
, vol. 
45
 
1
(pg. 
125
-
136
)
15.
Marmot
M
Brunner
E
Cohort profile: the Whitehall II study
Int J Epidemiol
 , 
2005
, vol. 
34
 
2
(pg. 
251
-
256
)
16.
Heim
AW
AH 4 Group Test of General Intelligence
 , 
1970
Windsor, United Kingdom
NFER-Nelson Publishing Company, Ltd
17.
Raven
JC
Guide to Using the Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale With Progressive Matrices
 , 
1965
London, United Kingdom
HK Lewis
18.
Borkowski
JG
Benton
AL
Spreen
O
Word fluency and brain damage
Neuropsychologia
 , 
1967
, vol. 
5
 
2
(pg. 
135
-
140
)
19.
Launer
LJ
Masaki
K
Petrovitch
H
, et al. 
The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study
JAMA
 , 
1995
, vol. 
274
 
23
(pg. 
1846
-
1851
)
20.
Breteler
MM
Vascular involvement in cognitive decline and dementia. Epidemiologic evidence from the Rotterdam Study and the Rotterdam Scan Study
Ann N Y Acad Sci.
 , 
2000
, vol. 
903
 (pg. 
457
-
465
)
21.
Singh-Manoux
A
Sabia
S
Lajnef
M
, et al. 
History of coronary heart disease and cognitive performance in midlife: the Whitehall II study
Eur Heart J
 , 
2008
, vol. 
29
 
17
(pg. 
2100
-
2107
)
22.
Swan
G
Lessov-Schlaggar
CN
The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain
Neuropsychol Rev.
 , 
2007
, vol. 
17
 
3
(pg. 
259
-
273
)
23.
Silvers
JM
Tokunaga
S
Berry
RB
, et al. 
Impairments in spatial learning and memory: ethanol, allopregnanolone, and the hippocampus
Brain Res Brain Res Rev.
 , 
2003
, vol. 
43
 
3
(pg. 
275
-
284
)
24.
Espeland
MA
Gu
L
Masaki
KH
, et al. 
Association between reported alcohol intake and cognition: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study
Am J Epidemiol
 , 
2005
, vol. 
161
 
3
(pg. 
228
-
238
)
25.
Britton
A
Singh-Manoux
A
Marmot
M
Alcohol consumption and cognitive function in the Whitehall II study
Am J Epidemiol
 , 
2004
, vol. 
160
 
3
(pg. 
240
-
247
)
26.
Colcombe
S
Kramer
AF
Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study
Psychol Sci.
 , 
2003
, vol. 
14
 
2
(pg. 
125
-
130
)
27.
Bassuk
SS
Berkman
LF
Wypij
D
Depressive symptomatology and incident cognitive decline in an elderly community sample
Arch Gen Psychiatry
 , 
1998
, vol. 
55
 
12
(pg. 
1073
-
1081
)
28.
Yaffe
K
Blackwell
T
Gore
R
, et al. 
Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in nondemented elderly women: a prospective study
Arch Gen Psychiatry
 , 
1999
, vol. 
56
 
5
(pg. 
425
-
430
)
29.
Philibert
I
Sleep loss and performance in residents and nonphysicians: a meta-analytic examination
Sleep
 , 
2005
, vol. 
28
 
11
(pg. 
1392
-
1402
)
30.
Singh-Manoux
A
Richards
M
Marmot
M
Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse: how does it relate to cognitive function in mid-life?
Ann Epidemiol
 , 
2005
, vol. 
15
 
8
(pg. 
572
-
578
)
31.
Lee
S
Buring
JE
Cook
NR
, et al. 
The relation of education and income to cognitive function among professional women
Neuroepidemiology
 , 
2006
, vol. 
26
 
2
(pg. 
93
-
101
)
32.
Koster
A
Penninx
BW
Bosma
H
, et al. 
Socioeconomic differences in cognitive decline and the role of biomedical factors
Ann Epidemiol
 , 
2005
, vol. 
15
 
8
(pg. 
564
-
571
)
33.
Glymour
MM
Weuve
J
Fay
ME
, et al. 
Social ties and cognitive recovery after stroke: does social integration promote cognitive resilience?
Neuroepidemiology
 , 
2008
, vol. 
31
 
1
(pg. 
10
-
20
)
34.
Bassuk
SS
Glass
TA
Berkman
LF
Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons
Ann Intern Med
 , 
1999
, vol. 
131
 
3
(pg. 
165
-
173
)
35.
Lee
S
Kawachi
I
Grodstein
F
Does caregiving stress affect cognitive function in older women?
J Nerv Ment Dis
 , 
2004
, vol. 
192
 
1
(pg. 
51
-
57
)
36.
Crowe
M
Andel
R
Pedersen
NL
, et al. 
Personality and risk of cognitive impairment 25 years later
Psychol Aging
 , 
2006
, vol. 
21
 
3
(pg. 
573
-
580
)
37.
Lupien
SJ
Maheu
F
Tu
M
, et al. 
The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition
Brain Cogn
 , 
2007
, vol. 
65
 
3
(pg. 
209
-
237
)
38.
Potter
GG
Helms
MJ
Plassman
BL
Associations of job demands and intelligence with cognitive performance among men in late life
Neurology
 , 
2008
, vol. 
70
 
19 pt 2
(pg. 
1803
-
1808
)
39.
Ware
JE
Jr
Kosinski
M
Bayliss
MS
, et al. 
Comparison of methods for the scoring and statistical analysis of SF-36 health profile and summary measures: summary of results from the Medical Outcomes Study
Med Care
 , 
1995
, vol. 
33
 
4 suppl
(pg. 
AS264
-
AS279
)
40.
Kivimäki
M
Head
J
Ferrie
JE
, et al. 
Hypertension is not the link between job strain and coronary heart disease in the Whitehall II study
Am J Hypertens
 , 
2007
, vol. 
20
 
11
(pg. 
1146
-
1153
)
41.
Goldberg
DP
The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire
 , 
1972
London, United Kingdom
Oxford University Press
42.
Stansfeld
SA
Marmot
MG
Social class and minor psychiatric disorder in British civil servants: a validated screening survey using the General Health Questionnaire
Psychol Med
 , 
1992
, vol. 
22
 
3
(pg. 
739
-
749
)
43.
Jenkins
R
Lewis
G
Bebbington
P
, et al. 
The National Psychiatric Morbidity Surveys of Great Britain—initial findings from the household survey
Psychol Med
 , 
1997
, vol. 
27
 
4
(pg. 
775
-
789
)
44.
Ferrie
JE
Shipley
MJ
Cappuccio
FP
, et al. 
A prospective study of change in sleep duration: associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort
Sleep
 , 
2007
, vol. 
30
 
12
(pg. 
1659
-
1666
)
45.
Jenkins
D
Stanton
BA
Niemcryk
S
, et al. 
A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research
J Clin Epidemiol
 , 
1988
, vol. 
41
 
4
(pg. 
313
-
321
)
46.
White
I
Altmann
DR
Nanchahal
K
Mortality in England and Wales attributable to any drinking, drinking above sensible limits and drinking above lowest risk level
Addiction
 , 
2004
, vol. 
99
 
6
(pg. 
749
-
756
)
47.
Kujala
UM
Sarna
S
Kaprio
J
, et al. 
Hospital care in later life among former world-class Finnish athletes
JAMA
 , 
1996
, vol. 
276
 
3
(pg. 
216
-
220
)
48.
Stansfeld
S
Marmot
MG
Deriving a survey measure of social support: the reliability and validity of the close persons questionnaire
Soc Sci Med
 , 
1992
, vol. 
35
 
8
(pg. 
1027
-
1035
)
49.
Karasek
RA
Job demands, job decision latitude and mental strain: implications for job redesign
Adm Sci Q
 , 
1979
, vol. 
24
 
2
(pg. 
285
-
308
)
50.
Glymour
M
Weuve
J
Berkman
LF
, et al. 
When is baseline adjustment useful in analyses of change? An example with education and cognitive function
Am J Epidemiol
 , 
2005
, vol. 
162
 
3
(pg. 
267
-
278
)
51.
Royston
P
Multiple imputation of missing values
Stata J
 , 
2004
, vol. 
4
 
3
(pg. 
227
-
241
)
52.
Christensen
H
What cognitive changes can be expected with normal ageing?
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
 , 
2001
, vol. 
35
 
6
(pg. 
768
-
775
)
53.
Blair
C
How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability
Behav Brain Sci.
 , 
2006
, vol. 
29
 
2
(pg. 
109
-
160
)
54.
Lundberg
U
Stress hormones in health and illness: the roles of work and gender
Psychoneuroendocrinology
 , 
2005
, vol. 
30
 
10
(pg. 
1017
-
1021
)
55.
Garde
AH
Faber
A
Persson
R
, et al. 
Concentrations of cortisol, testosterone and glycosylated haemoglobin (HbA1c) among construction workers with 12-h workdays and extended workweeks
Int Arch Occup Environ Health
 , 
2007
, vol. 
80
 
5
(pg. 
404
-
411
)
56.
Sabia
S
Guéguen
A
Marmot
MG
Does cognition predict mortality in midlife? Results from the Whitehall II cohort study
Neurobiol Aging
  
(doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.007)
57.
Pavlik
VN
de Moraes
SA
Szklo
M
, et al. 
Relation between cognitive function and mortality in middle-aged adults. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study
Am J Epidemiol
 , 
2003
, vol. 
157
 
4
(pg. 
327
-
334
)
58.
Portin
ML
Muuriaisniemi
M
Joukamaa
S
, et al. 
Cognitive impairment and the 10-year survival probability of a normal 62-year-old population
Scand J Psychol
 , 
2001
, vol. 
42
 
4
(pg. 
359
-
366
)
59.
Anstey
KJ
von Sanden
C
Salim
A
, et al. 
Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies
Am J Epidemiol
 , 
2007
, vol. 
166
 
4
(pg. 
367
-
378
)
60.
Sabia
S
Marmot
M
Dufouil
C
, et al. 
Smoking history and cognitive function in middle-age from the Whitehall II study
Arch Intern Med
 , 
2008
, vol. 
168
 
11
(pg. 
1165
-
1173
)